Year |
Citation |
Score |
2016 |
Ortells JJ, Noguera C, Álvarez D, Carmona E, Houghton G. Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity Modulates Semantic Negative Priming from Single Prime Words. Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 1286. PMID 27621716 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2016.01286 |
0.386 |
|
2013 |
Grange JA, Juvina I, Houghton G. On costs and benefits of n-2 repetitions in task switching: towards a behavioural marker of cognitive inhibition. Psychological Research. 77: 211-22. PMID 22327120 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-012-0421-4 |
0.686 |
|
2012 |
MarÃ-Beffa P, Cooper S, Houghton G. Unmixing the mixing cost: contributions from dimensional relevance and stimulus-response suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 478-88. PMID 22060145 DOI: 10.1037/A0025979 |
0.5 |
|
2011 |
Houghton G, Grange JA. CDF-XL: computing cumulative distribution functions of reaction time data in Excel. Behavior Research Methods. 43: 1023-32. PMID 21717268 DOI: 10.3758/S13428-011-0119-3 |
0.605 |
|
2011 |
Grange JA, Houghton G. Task preparation and task inhibition: a comment on Koch, Gade, Schuch, & Philipp (2010). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18: 211-6. PMID 21327359 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-010-0023-3 |
0.721 |
|
2010 |
Grange JA, Houghton G. Heightened conflict in cue-target translation increases backward inhibition in set switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 36: 1003-9. PMID 20565215 DOI: 10.1037/A0019129 |
0.696 |
|
2010 |
Grange JA, Houghton G. Cue-switch costs in task-switching: cue priming or control processes? Psychological Research. 74: 481-90. PMID 20037766 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-009-0270-Y |
0.693 |
|
2009 |
Houghton G, Pritchard R, Grange JA. The role of cue-target translation in backward inhibition of attentional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 35: 466-76. PMID 19271859 DOI: 10.1037/A0014648 |
0.717 |
|
2009 |
Grange JA, Houghton G. Temporal cue-target overlap is not essential for backward inhibition in task switching. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 62: 2068-79. PMID 19225989 DOI: 10.1080/17470210802696096 |
0.724 |
|
2009 |
Catena A, Houghton G, Valdés B, Fuentes LJ. Unmasking word processing with ERPs: two novel linear techniques for the estimation of temporally overlapped waveforms. Brain Topography. 22: 60-71. PMID 19194794 DOI: 10.1007/S10548-009-0074-9 |
0.325 |
|
2007 |
MarÃ-Beffa P, Catena A, Valdés B, Cullen D, Houghton G. N400, the reference electrode, and the semantic activation in prime-task experiments: a reply to Dombrowski and Heil (2006). Brain Research. 1147: 209-12. PMID 17328874 DOI: 10.1016/J.Brainres.2007.01.124 |
0.391 |
|
2005 |
MarÃ-Beffa P, Valdés B, Cullen DJ, Catena A, Houghton G. ERP analyses of task effects on semantic processing from words. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 23: 293-305. PMID 15820637 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogbrainres.2004.10.016 |
0.445 |
|
2003 |
Houghton G, Zorzi M. Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 20: 115-62. PMID 20957568 DOI: 10.1080/02643290242000871 |
0.302 |
|
2000 |
Milliken B, Tipper SP, Houghton G, Lupiáñez J. Attending, ignoring, and repetition: on the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics. 62: 1280-96. PMID 11019624 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03212130 |
0.374 |
|
2000 |
MarÃ-Beffa P, Fuentes LJ, Catena A, Houghton G. Semantic priming in the prime task effect: evidence of automatic semantic processing of distractors. Memory & Cognition. 28: 635-47. PMID 10946546 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03201253 |
0.465 |
|
2000 |
MarÃ-Beffa P, Houghton G, Estévez AF, Fuentes LJ. Word-based grouping affects the prime-task effect on semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 26: 469-79. PMID 10811157 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.26.2.469 |
0.449 |
|
1998 |
Tipper SP, Howard LA, Houghton G. Action-based mechanisms of attention. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 353: 1385-93. PMID 9770231 DOI: 10.1098/Rstb.1998.0292 |
0.344 |
|
1998 |
Zorzi M, Houghton G, Butterworth B. Two Routes or One in Reading Aloud? A Connectionist Dual-Process Model Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 24: 1131-1161. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.4.1131 |
0.35 |
|
1997 |
Baylis GC, Tipper SP, Houghton G. Externally cued and internally generated selection: differences in distractor analysis and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 23: 1617-30. PMID 9425671 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.23.6.1617 |
0.36 |
|
1996 |
Houghton G, Tipper SP, Weaver B, Shore DI. Inhibition and interference in selective attention: Some tests of a neural network model Visual Cognition. 3: 119-164. DOI: 10.1080/713756733 |
0.324 |
|
1996 |
Hartley T, Houghton G. A linguistically constrained model of short-term memory for nonwords Journal of Memory and Language. 35: 1-31. DOI: 10.1006/Jmla.1996.0001 |
0.498 |
|
1994 |
Tipper SP, Weaver B, Houghton G. Behavioural Goals Determine Inhibitory Mechanisms of Selective Attention The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 47: 809-840. DOI: 10.1080/14640749408401098 |
0.36 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |