Year |
Citation |
Score |
2023 |
Cheng C, Kibbe MM. Children's use of reasoning by exclusion to infer objects' identities in working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 237: 105765. PMID 37690346 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105765 |
0.399 |
|
2023 |
Kenderla P, Kim SH, Kibbe MM. Competition Between Object Topology and Surface Features in Children's Extension of Novel Nouns. Open Mind : Discoveries in Cognitive Science. 7: 93-110. PMID 37416072 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00073 |
0.346 |
|
2023 |
Cheng C, Kibbe MM. Is Nonsymbolic Arithmetic Truly "Arithmetic"? Examining the Computational Capacity of the Approximate Number System in Young Children. Cognitive Science. 47: e13299. PMID 37303302 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13299 |
0.319 |
|
2023 |
Blankenship TL, Kibbe MM. "Plan chunking" expands 3-year-olds' ability to complete multiple-step plans. Child Development. PMID 37092570 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13929 |
0.756 |
|
2022 |
Kenderla P, Kibbe MM. Explore versus store: Children strategically trade off reliance on exploration versus working memory during a complex task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 225: 105535. PMID 36041236 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105535 |
0.314 |
|
2022 |
Kibbe MM, Applin JB. Tracking what went where across toddlerhood: Feature-location bound object representations in 2- to 3-year-olds' working memory. Child Development. PMID 35716069 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13813 |
0.35 |
|
2022 |
Cheng C, Kibbe MM. Development of updating in working memory in 4-7-year-old children. Developmental Psychology. PMID 35311308 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001337 |
0.393 |
|
2021 |
Blankenship TL, Kibbe MM. Two-year-olds use past memories to accomplish novel goals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 214: 105286. PMID 34500114 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105286 |
0.733 |
|
2021 |
Smith-Flores AS, Applin JB, Blake PR, Kibbe MM. Children's understanding of economic demand: A dissociation between inference and choice. Cognition. 214: 104747. PMID 33971529 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104747 |
0.756 |
|
2020 |
Applin JB, Kibbe MM. Young children monitor the fidelity of visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 33151715 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000971 |
0.367 |
|
2020 |
Blankenship TL, Strong RW, Kibbe MM. Development of multiple object tracking via multifocal attention. Developmental Psychology. PMID 32614210 DOI: 10.1037/Dev0001064 |
0.788 |
|
2020 |
Blankenship T, Strong R, Kibbe M. Split foci of attention in middle childhood Journal of Vision. 20: 838. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.838 |
0.73 |
|
2019 |
Blankenship TL, Kibbe MM. Examining the limits of Memory-Guided Planning in 3- and 4-year olds. Cognitive Development. 52. PMID 32863569 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogdev.2019.100820 |
0.763 |
|
2019 |
Applin JB, Kibbe MM. Six-Month-Old Infants Predict Agents' Goal-Directed Actions on Occluded Objects. Infancy : the Official Journal of the International Society On Infant Studies. 24: 392-410. PMID 32677190 DOI: 10.1111/Infa.12282 |
0.407 |
|
2019 |
Gruen RL, Esfand SM, Kibbe MM. Altruistic self-regulation in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 104700. PMID 31623851 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jecp.2019.104700 |
0.651 |
|
2019 |
Kibbe MM, Leslie AM. Conceptually Rich, Perceptually Sparse: Object Representations in 6-Month-Old Infants' Working Memory. Psychological Science. 956797618817754. PMID 30668927 DOI: 10.1177/0956797618817754 |
0.683 |
|
2019 |
Kenderla PK, Kibbe MM. Visual working memory representations during a change detection task persist in long-term memory Journal of Vision. 19: 81a. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.81A |
0.331 |
|
2019 |
Blankenship TL, Strong RW, Kibbe MM. Development of children’s capacity for multiple object tracking via multifocal attention Journal of Vision. 19: 269b. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.269B |
0.749 |
|
2019 |
Ramirez LD, Schwartz J, Bloem I, Ling S, Kibbe MM. Spatial location does not elicit normalization in visual memory Journal of Vision. 19: 245. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.245 |
0.329 |
|
2018 |
St John AM, Kibbe M, Tarullo AR. A systematic assessment of socioeconomic status and executive functioning in early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. PMID 30292568 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jecp.2018.09.003 |
0.385 |
|
2018 |
Kibbe MM, Kaldy Z, Blaser E. Rules infants look by: Testing the assumption of transitivity in visual salience. Infancy : the Official Journal of the International Society On Infant Studies. 23: 156-172. PMID 29662430 DOI: 10.1111/Infa.12219 |
0.761 |
|
2018 |
Bloem IM, Watanabe YL, Kibbe MM, Ling S. Visual Memories Bypass Normalization. Psychological Science. 956797617747091. PMID 29596038 DOI: 10.1177/0956797617747091 |
0.337 |
|
2018 |
Blankenship T, Strong R, Kibbe M. Multiple object tracking via sustained multifocal attention in children Journal of Vision. 18: 780. DOI: 10.1167/18.10.780 |
0.79 |
|
2018 |
Kibbe MM, Kreisky M, Weisberg DS. Young children distinguish between different unrealistic fictional genres. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 12: 228-235. DOI: 10.1037/Aca0000115 |
0.441 |
|
2017 |
Kibbe MM, Feigenson L. A dissociation between small and large numbers in young children's ability to "solve for x" in non-symbolic math problems. Cognition. 160: 82-90. PMID 28068528 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2016.12.006 |
0.687 |
|
2017 |
Bloem I, Watanabe Y, Ling S, Kibbe M. Visual working memory representations bypass divisive normalization Journal of Vision. 17: 858. DOI: 10.1167/17.10.858 |
0.335 |
|
2016 |
Kibbe MM, Leslie AM. The ring that does not bind: Topological class in infants' working memory for objects Cognitive Development. 38: 1-9. DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogdev.2015.12.001 |
0.667 |
|
2015 |
Kibbe MM, Feigenson L. Infants use temporal regularities to chunk objects in memory. Cognition. 146: 251-263. PMID 26484498 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2015.09.022 |
0.677 |
|
2015 |
Kibbe MM, Feigenson L. Young children 'solve for x' using the Approximate Number System. Developmental Science. 18: 38-49. PMID 24589420 DOI: 10.1111/Desc.12177 |
0.651 |
|
2015 |
Kibbe MM. Varieties of Visual Working Memory Representation in Infancy and Beyond Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 433-439. DOI: 10.1177/0963721415605831 |
0.385 |
|
2015 |
Kibbe M. Visual working memory for multiple moving objects in occlusion Journal of Vision. 15: 545. DOI: 10.1167/15.12.545 |
0.382 |
|
2014 |
Kibbe MM, Feigenson L. Developmental origins of recoding and decoding in memory. Cognitive Psychology. 75: 55-79. PMID 25195153 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2014.08.001 |
0.687 |
|
2013 |
Kibbe MM, Leslie AM. What's the object of object working memory in infancy? Unraveling 'what' and 'how many'. Cognitive Psychology. 66: 380-404. PMID 23770623 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2013.05.001 |
0.684 |
|
2013 |
Kibbe M, Feigenson L. Infants use statistical regularities to chunk items in visual working memory. Journal of Vision. 13: 333-333. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.333 |
0.656 |
|
2011 |
Kibbe MM, Leslie AM. What do infants remember when they forget? Location and identity in 6-month-olds' memory for objects. Psychological Science. 22: 1500-5. PMID 22095976 DOI: 10.1177/0956797611420165 |
0.669 |
|
2011 |
Kibbe MM, Kowler E. Visual search for category sets: tradeoffs between exploration and memory. Journal of Vision. 11. PMID 21421747 DOI: 10.1167/11.3.14 |
0.352 |
|
2010 |
Kibbe M. The complexity of a category affects working memory capacity in a search task Journal of Vision. 8: 1171-1171. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.1171 |
0.309 |
|
2010 |
Kaldy Z, Blaser E, Kibbe M. Detection vs. Saliance of color and motion-defiend stimuli in 6-month-old infants Journal of Vision. 6: 292-292. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.292 |
0.736 |
|
2010 |
Kaldy Z, Blaser E, Kibbe M, Pomplun M. What drives visual salience in young infants? Journal of Vision. 5: 512-512. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.512 |
0.742 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |