Year |
Citation |
Score |
2019 |
Kanaya HK, Morita MM, Sato T. Examination of Lower Level Motion Mechanisms That Provide Information to Object Tracking: An Examination Using Dichoptic Stimulation. I-Perception. 10: 2041669519891745. PMID 31832128 DOI: 10.1177/2041669519891745 |
0.483 |
|
2018 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. Discretized Theta-Rhythm Perception Revealed by Moving Stimuli. Scientific Reports. 8: 5682. PMID 29632313 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-24131-6 |
0.368 |
|
2016 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. The Roles of Non-retinotopic Motions in Visual Search. Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 840. PMID 27313560 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00840 |
0.514 |
|
2016 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. Motion dominance in binocular rivalry depends on extraretinal motions. Journal of Vision. 16: 2. PMID 26943347 DOI: 10.1167/16.5.2 |
0.531 |
|
2015 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. Directional asymmetry in contrast sensitivity during smooth pursuit eye movement depends on spatial frequency. Journal of Vision. 15: 1021. PMID 26326709 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.1021 |
0.318 |
|
2014 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. Competing motion signals compromise to discrete perception Journal of Vision. 14: 264-264. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.264 |
0.478 |
|
2013 |
Hosokawa K, Maruya K, Sato T. Temporal characteristics of depth perception from motion parallax. Journal of Vision. 13. PMID 23308024 DOI: 10.1167/13.1.16 |
0.678 |
|
2013 |
Kanaya H, Sato T. Lower visual field advantage in multiple object tracking is mediated by a non-attentive motion mechanism Journal of Vision. 13: 967-967. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.967 |
0.456 |
|
2013 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. Motion pop-out is determined by extra-retinal coordinate Journal of Vision. 13: 964-964. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.964 |
0.447 |
|
2013 |
Sato H, Motoyoshi I, Sato T. Perception of global trend from dynamic stimuli Journal of Vision. 13: 765-765. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.765 |
0.326 |
|
2013 |
Sato T, Kanaya H, Fujita M. Spatio-temporal characteristics of classical apparent motion traversing vertical- and horizontal meridians Journal of Vision. 13: 370-370. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.370 |
0.51 |
|
2012 |
Kuwahara M, Sato T, Yotsumoto Y. Wriggling motion trajectory illusion. Journal of Vision. 12: 4. PMID 23139160 DOI: 10.1167/12.12.4 |
0.691 |
|
2012 |
Kanaya H, Sato T. Contribution of nonattentive motion to object tracking. Journal of Vision. 12. PMID 23115216 DOI: 10.1167/12.11.28 |
0.492 |
|
2012 |
Nakajima Y, Sato T, Hara K, Yotsumoto Y. Shape-From-Motion is intact even when motion perception is impaired: a TMS study Journal of Vision. 12: 757-757. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.757 |
0.723 |
|
2012 |
Murofushi Y, Ono K, Sato T, Kitazaki M. Perceived direction of human, robot and point-light walkers modulated by head direction Journal of Vision. 12: 464-464. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.464 |
0.58 |
|
2012 |
Nakayama R, Motoyoshi I, Kusano T, Sato T. Spatial motion coordinates that determine perceptual dominance in binocular rivalry Journal of Vision. 12: 1264-1264. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1264 |
0.506 |
|
2012 |
Shigemasu H, Sato T. Effects of the amount of monocular shape information on stereo scaling problem Japanese Psychological Research. 54: 27-37. DOI: 10.1111/J.1468-5884.2011.00509.X |
0.424 |
|
2012 |
Seno T, Sato T. Vection can be induced without explicit motion signal using backscroll illusion Japanese Psychological Research. 54: 218-222. DOI: 10.1111/J.1468-5884.2011.00498.X |
0.523 |
|
2012 |
Sato T, Hosokawa K. Mona Lisa Effect of Eyes and Face I-Perception. 3: 707-707. DOI: 10.1068/If707 |
0.317 |
|
2011 |
Ono K, Sato T, Kitazaki M. Search asymmetry in perceiving walkers: Inversion effect and biological motion stimuli Journal of Vision. 11: 694-694. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.694 |
0.712 |
|
2010 |
Maruya K, Kanai R, Sato T. Motion of motion-defined pattern does not induce spatial mislocalization Journal of Vision. 8: 597-597. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.597 |
0.703 |
|
2010 |
Inokuma A, Sato T. Induced motion with chromatic stimuli Journal of Vision. 8: 124-124. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.124 |
0.507 |
|
2010 |
Nakajima Y, Sato T. The involvement of local motion adaptation in global motion aftereffect Journal of Vision. 8: 1036-1036. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.1036 |
0.494 |
|
2010 |
Isogaya Y, Maruya K, Nakajima Y, Tani Y, Sato T. Self-range defined by gaze perception is robust against the size and viewing distance variations Journal of Vision. 7: 17-17. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.17 |
0.585 |
|
2010 |
Harasawa M, Obata A, Morita T, Ito T, Saito T, Sato T, Aizawa K. Hemodynamic changes in visual motion detection measured by near infrared spectroscopy Journal of Vision. 6: 572-572. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.572 |
0.763 |
|
2010 |
Hosokawa K, Sato T. Shearing and compressive motions work cooperatively to reconstruct structure from motion Journal of Vision. 6: 370-370. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.370 |
0.466 |
|
2010 |
Isogaya Y, Maruya K, Nakajima Y, Tani Y, Sato T. A self-range defined by gaze perception affected by characteristics of personality Journal of Vision. 6: 281-281. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.281 |
0.573 |
|
2010 |
Maruya K, Nakajima Y, Sato T. Processing time of second-order contour formation Journal of Vision. 6: 207-207. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.207 |
0.538 |
|
2010 |
Seno T, Sato T. Temporonasal motion induces stronger vection Journal of Vision. 6: 148-148. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.148 |
0.494 |
|
2010 |
Matsuzaki N, Sato T. Facial expressions can be perceived from second-order motion Journal of Vision. 6: 1060-1060. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.1060 |
0.454 |
|
2010 |
Nakajima Y, Sato T. Spatial selectivity of local motion affects global motion after-effect Journal of Vision. 6: 1048-1048. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.1048 |
0.497 |
|
2010 |
Seno T, Sato T. The direction of vection is controlled by perceived motion Journal of Vision. 5: 319-319. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.319 |
0.502 |
|
2010 |
Kanaya H, Maruya K, Sato T. The contribution of low-level motion systems in multiple object tracking Journal of Vision. 5: 144-144. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.144 |
0.663 |
|
2010 |
Maruya K, Sato T. A contribution of early motion systems on stream-bounce perception Journal of Vision. 5: 138-138. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.138 |
0.687 |
|
2010 |
Tani Y, Nakajima Y, Maruya K, Sato T. The role of the visual feedback on the pointing behavior Journal of Vision. 5: 129-129. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.129 |
0.565 |
|
2010 |
Maruya K, Sato T. Perceptual offset between first- and second-order motion stimulus Journal of Vision. 3: 803-803. DOI: 10.1167/3.9.803 |
0.697 |
|
2010 |
Harasawa M, Sato T. The critical factor for performance improvement in multi-frame orientation texture segregation Journal of Vision. 3: 616-616. DOI: 10.1167/3.9.616 |
0.719 |
|
2010 |
Tani Y, Maruya K, Sato T. Reversed Cafe wall illusion Journal of Vision. 3: 351-351. DOI: 10.1167/3.9.351 |
0.568 |
|
2010 |
Sato T, Kayahara T. Visual capture of auditory motion Journal of Vision. 2: 666-666. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.666 |
0.484 |
|
2010 |
Harasawa M, Maruya K, Sato T. Perception of motion with orientation-defined missing fundamental gratings Journal of Vision. 2: 640-640. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.640 |
0.776 |
|
2010 |
Ohtsuka S, Sato T. Does depth from motion pop-out? Journal of Vision. 2: 385-385. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.385 |
0.513 |
|
2010 |
Fujimoto K, Sato T. Motion induction by biological motion Journal of Vision. 2: 337-337. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.337 |
0.505 |
|
2010 |
Onimaru S, Sato T, Kitazaki M. Veridical walking inhibits vection perception Journal of Vision. 10: 860-860. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.860 |
0.609 |
|
2010 |
Sato T, Nakajima Y. Orientation and shape tuning of van Lier aftereffect Journal of Vision. 10: 1392-1392. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1392 |
0.309 |
|
2010 |
Kamiya S, Sato T. Motion- and luminance- defined patterns elicit qualitatively same but quantitatively different VEPs Journal of Vision. 1: 245-245. DOI: 10.1167/1.3.245 |
0.479 |
|
2009 |
Fujimoto K, Yagi A, Sato T. Strength and variability of the backscroll illusion. Vision Research. 49: 759-64. PMID 19236893 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.006 |
0.486 |
|
2009 |
Seno T, Sato T. Positional and directional preponderances in vection. Experimental Brain Research. 192: 221-9. PMID 18818907 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-008-1575-6 |
0.412 |
|
2008 |
Matsuzaki N, Sato T. The perception of facial expressions from two-frame apparent motion. Perception. 37: 1560-8. PMID 19065858 DOI: 10.1068/P5769 |
0.449 |
|
2008 |
Sato T, Inoue Y, Tani T, Matsuzaki N, Kawamura K, Kitazaki M. Perceiving the direction of walking Perception. 37: 27-27. DOI: 10.1068/V080096 |
0.642 |
|
2007 |
Sato T, Seno T, Kanaya H, Fukazawa H. Dominance of ground stimuli in vection induction Perception. 36: 0-0. DOI: 10.1068/V070045 |
0.313 |
|
2007 |
Fujimoto K, Sato T. Erratum to “Backscroll illusion: Apparent motion in the background of locomotive objects” [Vis. Res. 46 (2006) 14–25] Vision Research. 47: 1410. DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2006.01.036 |
0.489 |
|
2006 |
Tani Y, Maruya K, Sato T. Reversed Café Wall illusion with missing fundamental gratings. Vision Research. 46: 3782-5. PMID 16938329 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2006.07.002 |
0.603 |
|
2006 |
Fujimoto K, Sato T. Backscroll illusion: apparent motion in the background of locomotive objects. Vision Research. 46: 14-25. PMID 16289275 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.09.027 |
0.54 |
|
2005 |
Sato T, Maruya K. Local motion speed affects the perceived speed of motion-defined motion Journal of Vision. 5: 930-930. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.930 |
0.711 |
|
2005 |
Hosokawa K, Ohtsuka S, Sato T. Depth perception from intermittent motion parallax stimuli Journal of Vision. 5: 729-729. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.729 |
0.537 |
|
2004 |
Maruya K, Sato T. A dichotomy in representation for locations of moving objects. Journal of Vision. 4: 578-578. DOI: 10.1167/4.8.578 |
0.597 |
|
2003 |
Maruya K, Mugishima Y, Sato T. Reversed-phi perception with motion-defined motion stimuli. Vision Research. 43: 2517-26. PMID 13129539 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00438-3 |
0.727 |
|
2003 |
Kitazaki M, Sato T. Attentional modulation of self-motion perception. Perception. 32: 475-84. PMID 12785485 DOI: 10.1068/P5037 |
0.724 |
|
2002 |
Shioiri S, Ono H, Sato T. Adaptation to relative and uniform motion. Journal of the Optical Society of America. a, Optics, Image Science, and Vision. 19: 1465-74. PMID 12152686 DOI: 10.1364/Josaa.19.001465 |
0.741 |
|
2001 |
Fujimoto K, Yagi A, Sato T. Incompatible body-translation delays visual perception of human gait Journal of Vision. 1. DOI: 10.1167/1.3.352 |
0.499 |
|
2001 |
Kawashima T, Sato T. Auditory motion aftereffects with varying interaural phase difference The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 109: 2377-2377. DOI: 10.1121/1.4744375 |
0.505 |
|
1996 |
Kawahara J, Yokosawa K, Nishida S, Sato T. Illusory line motion in visual search: attentional facilitation or apparent motion? Perception. 25: 901-20. PMID 8938004 DOI: 10.1068/p250901 |
0.761 |
|
1994 |
Nishida S, Sato T. Special Edition. Pattern Recognition and Image Understanding. 4. Motion Detection Mechanisms in the Human Visual System. The Journal of the Institute of Television Engineers of Japan. 48: 157-163. DOI: 10.3169/itej1978.48.157 |
0.7 |
|
1993 |
Shioiri S, Sato T. Influence of Depth from Contour Shape on Binocular Stereopsis. The Journal of the Institute of Television Engineers of Japan. 47: 364-370. DOI: 10.3169/itej1978.47.364 |
0.664 |
|
1991 |
Shioiri S, Sato T. Special issue. Depth perception and three dimensional imaging. 2. Depth perception. 2-1. Binocular stereopsis and monocular vision. The Journal of the Institute of Television Engineers of Japan. 45: 431-437. DOI: 10.3169/itej1978.45.431 |
0.654 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |