Year |
Citation |
Score |
2024 |
Quinn J, Goldrick M, Arnett C, Ferreira VS, Gollan TH. Syntax drives default language selection in bilingual connected speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 39418449 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001405 |
0.586 |
|
2024 |
Gollan TH, Garcia DL, Murillo M, Vargas J, Pulido B, Salmon DP. Sprinting in two languages: Picture naming performance of older Spanish-English bilinguals on the Multilingual Naming Test Sprint 2.0. Neuropsychology. PMID 38990683 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000958 |
0.556 |
|
2024 |
Neveu A, Goldrick M, Kleinman D, Salmon DP, Gollan TH. Revisiting which language declines more in Spanish-English bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease: Longitudinal decline patterns on the multilingual naming test. Neuropsychologia. 202: 108948. PMID 38971370 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108948 |
0.588 |
|
2024 |
Bannon J, Gollan TH, Ferreira VS. Is predicting during language processing worth it? Effects of cloze probability and semantic similarity on failed predictions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 38683551 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001347 |
0.366 |
|
2024 |
Neveu A, Gollan TH. New insights on what leads bilinguals to be able to name some pictures only in their nondominant language: Immersion, dominance reversal, and balanced bilingualism. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 1-7. PMID 38369517 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617724000067 |
0.598 |
|
2024 |
Garcia DL, Gollan TH. Language switching and speaking a nondominant language challenge executive control: Preliminary data for novel behavioral markers of Alzheimer's risk in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology. PMID 38330361 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000943 |
0.474 |
|
2023 |
Li C, Midgley KJ, Ferreira VS, Holcomb PJ, Gollan TH. Different language control mechanisms in comprehension and production: Evidence from paragraph reading. Brain and Language. 248: 105367. PMID 38113600 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2023.105367 |
0.611 |
|
2023 |
Bannon J, Ferreira VS, Stasenko A, Gollan TH. Competition accumulates in successive retrieval of proper names. Memory & Cognition. PMID 37721701 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01455-x |
0.322 |
|
2023 |
Gollan TH, Stasenko A, Salmon DP. Which language is more affected in bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease? Diagnostic sensitivity of the Multilingual Naming Test. Neuropsychology. PMID 36931816 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000893 |
0.405 |
|
2023 |
Goldrick M, Gollan TH. Inhibitory control of the dominant language: Reversed language dominance is the tip of the iceberg. Journal of Memory and Language. 130. PMID 36873561 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104410 |
0.613 |
|
2022 |
Gollan TH, Stasenko A, Li C, Smirnov DS, Galasko D, Salmon DP. Autocorrection if→of function words in reading aloud: A novel marker of Alzheimer's risk. Neuropsychology. PMID 35925735 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000829 |
0.337 |
|
2021 |
Li C, Gollan TH. Language-switch Costs from Comprehension to Production Might Just Be Task-switch Costs. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England). 25: 459-470. PMID 35669733 DOI: 10.1017/s1366728921001061 |
0.56 |
|
2021 |
Garcia DL, Gollan TH. The MINT Sprint: Exploring a Fast Administration Procedure with an Expanded Multilingual Naming Test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 1-17. PMID 34463235 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617721001004 |
0.484 |
|
2021 |
Stasenko A, Kleinman D, Gollan TH. Older bilinguals reverse language dominance less than younger bilinguals: Evidence for the inhibitory deficit hypothesis. Psychology and Aging. PMID 34166027 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000618 |
0.599 |
|
2021 |
Stasenko A, Kleinman D, Gollan TH. Older bilinguals reverse language dominance less than younger bilinguals: Evidence for the inhibitory deficit hypothesis. Psychology and Aging. PMID 34166027 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000618 |
0.599 |
|
2021 |
Mizrahi R, Wixted JT, Gollan TH. Order effects in bilingual recognition memory partially confirm predictions of the frequency-lag hypothesis. Memory (Hove, England). 1-12. PMID 33783316 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1902538 |
0.463 |
|
2021 |
Segal D, Prior A, Gollan TH. Do All Switches Cost the Same? Reliability of Language Switching and Mixing Costs. Journal of Cognition. 4: 3. PMID 33506169 DOI: 10.5334/joc.140 |
0.416 |
|
2021 |
Li C, Gollan TH. What Cognates Reveal about Default Language Selection in Bilingual Sentence Production. Journal of Memory and Language. 118. PMID 33456132 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104214 |
0.6 |
|
2020 |
Tomoschuk B, Ferreira VS, Gollan TH. Translation Distractors Facilitate Production in Single- and Mixed-Language Picture Naming. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. 36: 854-866. PMID 35706503 DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2020.1852291 |
0.611 |
|
2020 |
Suárez PA, Marquine MJ, Díaz-Santos M, Gollan T, Artiola I Fortuny L, Rivera Mindt M, Heaton R, Cherner M. Native Spanish-speaker's test performance and the effects of Spanish-English bilingualism: results from the neuropsychological norms for the U.S.-Mexico Border Region in Spanish (NP-NUMBRS) project. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1-13. PMID 33356892 DOI: 10.1080/13854046.2020.1861330 |
0.412 |
|
2020 |
Declerck M, Kleinman D, Gollan TH. Which bilinguals reverse language dominance and why? Cognition. 204: 104384. PMID 32634738 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2020.104384 |
0.643 |
|
2020 |
Gollan TH, Smirnov DS, Salmon DP, Galasko D. Failure to stop autocorrect errors in reading aloud increases in aging especially with a positive biomarker for Alzheimer's disease. Psychology and Aging. PMID 32584071 DOI: 10.1037/Pag0000550 |
0.433 |
|
2020 |
Ahn D, Abbott MJ, Rayner K, Ferreira VS, Gollan TH. Minimal Overlap in Language Control Across Production And Comprehension: Evidence from Read-Aloud Versus Eye-Tracking Tasks. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 54. PMID 32189830 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jneuroling.2019.100885 |
0.655 |
|
2020 |
Stasenko A, Hays C, Wierenga CE, Gollan TH. Cognitive control regions are recruited in bilinguals' silent reading of mixed-language paragraphs. Brain and Language. 204: 104754. PMID 32113072 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandl.2020.104754 |
0.592 |
|
2019 |
Gollan TH, Li C, Stasenko A, Salmon DP. Intact reversed language-dominance but exaggerated cognate effects in reading aloud of language switches in bilingual Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. PMID 31545627 DOI: 10.1037/Neu0000592 |
0.621 |
|
2019 |
Fadlon J, Li C, Prior A, Gollan TH. Using what's there: Bilinguals adaptively rely on orthographic and color cues to achieve language control. Cognition. 191: 103990. PMID 31376660 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2019.06.002 |
0.638 |
|
2019 |
Stasenko A, Gollan TH. Tip of the tongue after any language: Reintroducing the notion of blocked retrieval. Cognition. 193: 104027. PMID 31369922 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2019.104027 |
0.645 |
|
2019 |
Smirnov DS, Stasenko A, Salmon DP, Galasko D, Brewer JB, Gollan TH. Distinct structural correlates of the dominant and nondominant languages in bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease (AD). Neuropsychologia. 132: 107131. PMID 31271821 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2019.107131 |
0.587 |
|
2019 |
Emmorey K, Li C, Petrich J, Gollan TH. Turning languages on and off: Switching into and out of code-blends reveals the nature of bilingual language control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 31246060 DOI: 10.1037/Xlm0000734 |
0.641 |
|
2019 |
Weissberger GH, Gollan TH, Bondi MW, Nation DA, Hansen LA, Galasko D, Salmon DP. Neuropsychological Deficit Profiles, Vascular Risk Factors, and Neuropathological Findings in Hispanic Older Adults with Autopsy-Confirmed Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease : Jad. 67: 291-302. PMID 30636736 DOI: 10.3233/Jad-180351 |
0.309 |
|
2019 |
Tao L, Cai Q, Gollan TH. Effects of cumulative language exposure on heritage and majority language skills: Spanish and Mandarin heritage speakers in the USA Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. DOI: 10.1075/Lab.18044.Tao |
0.505 |
|
2018 |
Schotter ER, Li C, Gollan T. Author accepted manuscript: What Reading Aloud Reveals about Speaking: Regressive saccades implicate a failure to monitor, not inattention, in the prevalence of intrusion errors on function words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 1747021818819480. PMID 30509156 DOI: 10.1177/1747021818819480 |
0.551 |
|
2018 |
Segal D, Stasenko A, Gollan TH. More evidence that a switch is not (always) a switch: Binning bilinguals reveals dissociations between task and language switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. PMID 30394767 DOI: 10.1037/Xge0000515 |
0.454 |
|
2018 |
Gollan TH, Goldrick M. Aging deficits in naturalistic speech production and monitoring revealed through reading aloud. Psychology and Aging. PMID 30265018 DOI: 10.1037/Pag0000296 |
0.489 |
|
2018 |
Segal D, Gollan TH. What's left for balanced bilinguals? Language proficiency and item familiarity affect left-hemisphere specialization in metaphor processing. Neuropsychology. PMID 30160502 DOI: 10.1037/Neu0000467 |
0.551 |
|
2018 |
Li C, Gollan TH. Cognates interfere with language selection but enhance monitoring in connected speech. Memory & Cognition. PMID 29679293 DOI: 10.3758/S13421-018-0812-X |
0.649 |
|
2018 |
Kleinman D, Gollan TH. Inhibition accumulates over time at multiple processing levels in bilingual language control. Cognition. 173: 115-132. PMID 29405945 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2018.01.009 |
0.644 |
|
2018 |
TOMOSCHUK B, FERREIRA VS, GOLLAN TH. When a seven is not a seven: Self-ratings of bilingual language proficiency differ between and within language populations Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 22: 516-536. DOI: 10.1017/S1366728918000421 |
0.482 |
|
2017 |
Li C, Gollan TH. Cognates Facilitate Switches and Then Confusion: Contrasting Effects of Cascade Versus Feedback on Language Selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 29283605 DOI: 10.1037/Xlm0000497 |
0.623 |
|
2017 |
Gollan TH, Goldrick M. A Switch is Not a Switch: Syntactically-Driven Bilingual Language Control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 28782969 DOI: 10.1037/Xlm0000462 |
0.654 |
|
2017 |
Gollan TH, Stasenko A, Li C, Salmon DP. Bilingual language intrusions and other speech errors in Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Cognition. 118: 27-44. PMID 28753438 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2017.07.007 |
0.659 |
|
2017 |
Stasenko A, Matt GE, Gollan TH. A Relative Bilingual Advantage in Switching With Preparation: Nuanced Explorations of the Proposed Association Between Bilingualism and Task Switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. PMID 28714710 DOI: 10.1037/Xge0000340 |
0.432 |
|
2017 |
Ivanova I, Ferreira VS, Gollan TH. Form Overrides Meaning When Bilinguals Monitor for Errors. Journal of Memory and Language. 94: 75-102. PMID 28649169 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2016.11.004 |
0.635 |
|
2017 |
Li C, Goldrick M, Gollan TH. Bilinguals' twisted tongues: Frequency lag or interference? Memory & Cognition. PMID 28265900 DOI: 10.3758/S13421-017-0688-1 |
0.587 |
|
2016 |
Emmorey K, Giezen MR, Gollan TH. Psycholinguistic, cognitive, and neural implications of bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England). 19: 223-242. PMID 28804269 DOI: 10.1017/S1366728915000085 |
0.633 |
|
2016 |
Emmorey K, Giezen MR, Gollan TH. Insights from bimodal bilingualism: Reply to commentaries. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England). 19: 261-263. PMID 28781571 DOI: 10.1017/S136672891500070X |
0.489 |
|
2016 |
Ivanova I, Murillo M, Montoya RI, Gollan TH. Does Bilingual Language Control Decline in Older Age? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. 6: 86-118. PMID 28090222 DOI: 10.1075/Lab.15003.Iva |
0.634 |
|
2016 |
Gollan TH, Goldrick M. Grammatical Constraints on Language Switching: Language Control is not Just Executive Control. Journal of Memory and Language. 90: 177-199. PMID 27667899 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2016.04.002 |
0.651 |
|
2016 |
Kleinman D, Gollan TH. Speaking Two Languages for the Price of One: Bypassing Language Control Mechanisms via Accessibility-Driven Switches. Psychological Science. PMID 27016240 DOI: 10.1177/0956797616634633 |
0.644 |
|
2016 |
Ivanova I, Murillo M, Montoya RI, Gollan TH. Does bilingual language control decline in older age? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. 6: 86-118. DOI: 10.1075/lab.15003.iva |
0.574 |
|
2016 |
Van Assche E, Duyck W, Gollan TH. Linking recognition and production: Cross-modal transfer effects between picture naming and lexical decision during first and second language processing in bilinguals Journal of Memory and Language. 89: 37-54. DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2016.02.003 |
0.511 |
|
2015 |
Tao L, Taft M, Gollan TH. The Bilingual Switching Advantage: Sometimes Related to Bilingual Proficiency, Sometimes Not. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 21: 531-44. PMID 26527242 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617715000521 |
0.638 |
|
2015 |
Weissberger GH, Gollan TH, Bondi MW, Clark LR, Wierenga CE. Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts. Neuropsychologia. 66: 193-203. PMID 25446970 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2014.10.037 |
0.583 |
|
2015 |
Gollan TH, Starr J, Ferreira VS. More than use it or lose it: the number-of-speakers effect on heritage language proficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 22: 147-55. PMID 24942146 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-014-0649-7 |
0.64 |
|
2015 |
EMMOREY K, GIEZEN MR, GOLLAN TH. Insights from bimodal bilingualism: Reply to commentaries Bilingualism. DOI: 10.1017/S136672891500070X |
0.36 |
|
2014 |
Sheng L, Lu Y, Gollan TH. Assessing language dominance in Mandarin-English bilinguals: Convergence and divergence between subjective and objective measures. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England). 17: 364-383. PMID 25379011 DOI: 10.1017/S1366728913000424 |
0.494 |
|
2014 |
Gollan TH, Kleinman D, Wierenga CE. What's easier: doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 143: 2167-95. PMID 25313951 DOI: 10.1037/A0038006 |
0.444 |
|
2014 |
Ivanova I, Salmon DP, Gollan TH. Which language declines more? longitudinal versus cross-sectional decline of picture naming in bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 20: 534-46. PMID 24725624 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617714000228 |
0.525 |
|
2014 |
Gollan TH, Ferreira VS, Cera C, Flett S. Translation-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states. Language and Cognitive Processes. 29: 278-288. PMID 24644375 DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2012.762457 |
0.63 |
|
2014 |
Suarez PA, Gollan TH, Heaton R, Grant I, Cherner M. Second-language fluency predicts native language stroop effects: evidence from Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 20: 342-8. PMID 24622502 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617714000058 |
0.654 |
|
2014 |
Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners: evidence from speakers with Alzheimer׳s disease. Neuropsychologia. 56: 184-95. PMID 24467889 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2014.01.013 |
0.388 |
|
2014 |
Gollan TH, Schotter ER, Gomez J, Murillo M, Rayner K. Multiple levels of bilingual language control: evidence from language intrusions in reading aloud. Psychological Science. 25: 585-95. PMID 24367061 DOI: 10.1177/0956797613512661 |
0.664 |
|
2013 |
Prior A, Gollan TH. The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer. Journal of Cognitive Psychology (Hove, England). 25: 622-645. PMID 24688756 DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2013.821993 |
0.536 |
|
2013 |
Runnqvist E, Gollan TH, Costa A, Ferreira VS. A disadvantage in bilingual sentence production modulated by syntactic frequency and similarity across languages. Cognition. 129: 256-63. PMID 23948209 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2013.07.008 |
0.622 |
|
2013 |
Weissberger GH, Salmon DP, Bondi MW, Gollan TH. Which neuropsychological tests predict progression to Alzheimer's disease in Hispanics? Neuropsychology. 27: 343-55. PMID 23688216 DOI: 10.1037/A0032399 |
0.347 |
|
2013 |
Kang SH, Gollan TH, Pashler H. Don't just repeat after me: retrieval practice is better than imitation for foreign vocabulary learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 20: 1259-65. PMID 23681928 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-013-0450-Z |
0.469 |
|
2013 |
Van Assche E, Duyck W, Gollan TH. Whole-language and item-specific control in bilingual language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 39: 1781-92. PMID 23647380 DOI: 10.1037/A0032859 |
0.668 |
|
2013 |
Ivanova I, Salmon DP, Gollan TH. The multilingual naming test in Alzheimer's disease: clues to the origin of naming impairments. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 19: 272-83. PMID 23298442 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617712001282 |
0.51 |
|
2013 |
Emmorey K, Petrich JA, Gollan TH. Bimodal bilingualism and the frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 18: 1-11. PMID 23073709 DOI: 10.1093/Deafed/Ens034 |
0.622 |
|
2012 |
Gollan TH, Weissberger GH, Runnqvist E, Montoya RI, Cera CM. Self-ratings of Spoken Language Dominance: A Multi-Lingual Naming Test (MINT) and Preliminary Norms for Young and Aging Spanish-English Bilinguals. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England). 15: 594-615. PMID 25364296 DOI: 10.1017/S1366728911000332 |
0.559 |
|
2012 |
Gollan TH, Goldrick M. Does bilingualism twist your tongue? Cognition. 125: 491-7. PMID 22959222 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2012.08.002 |
0.571 |
|
2012 |
Emmorey K, Petrich J, Gollan TH. Bilingual processing of ASL-English code-blends: The consequences of accessing two lexical representations simultaneously. Journal of Memory and Language. 67: 199-210. PMID 22773886 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2012.04.005 |
0.501 |
|
2012 |
Weissberger GH, Wierenga CE, Bondi MW, Gollan TH. Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging. 27: 959-74. PMID 22582883 DOI: 10.1037/A0028281 |
0.55 |
|
2012 |
Antón-Méndez I, Schütze CT, Champion MK, Gollan TH. What the tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) says about homophone frequency inheritance. Memory & Cognition. 40: 802-11. PMID 22351522 DOI: 10.3758/S13421-012-0189-1 |
0.313 |
|
2012 |
Kamat R, Ghate M, Gollan TH, Meyer R, Vaida F, Heaton RK, Letendre S, Franklin D, Alexander T, Grant I, Mehendale S, Marcotte TD. Effects of Marathi-Hindi bilingualism on neuropsychological performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 18: 305-13. PMID 22206622 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617711001731 |
0.603 |
|
2011 |
Silverberg NB, Ryan LM, Carrillo MC, Sperling R, Petersen RC, Posner HB, Snyder PJ, Hilsabeck R, Gallagher M, Raber J, Rizzo A, Possin K, King J, Kaye J, Ott BR, ... ... Gollan TH, et al. Assessment of cognition in early dementia. Alzheimer's & Dementia : the Journal of the Alzheimer's Association. 7: e60-e76. PMID 23559893 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jalz.2011.05.001 |
0.319 |
|
2011 |
Prior A, Gollan TH. Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 17: 682-91. PMID 22882810 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617711000580 |
0.61 |
|
2011 |
Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Montoya RI, Galasko DR. Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia. 49: 3826-30. PMID 22001315 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2011.09.041 |
0.447 |
|
2011 |
Gollan TH, Sandoval T, Salmon DP. Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science. 22: 1155-64. PMID 21775653 DOI: 10.1177/0956797611417002 |
0.642 |
|
2011 |
Gollan TH, Slattery TJ, Goldenberg D, Van Assche E, Duyck W, Rayner K. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: the frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 140: 186-209. PMID 21219080 DOI: 10.1037/A0022256 |
0.44 |
|
2010 |
Antón-Méndez I, Gollan TH. Not just semantics: strong frequency and weak cognate effects on semantic association in bilinguals. Memory & Cognition. 38: 723-39. PMID 20852236 DOI: 10.3758/Mc.38.6.723 |
0.475 |
|
2010 |
Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Montoya RI, da Pena E. Accessibility of the nondominant language in picture naming: a counterintuitive effect of dementia on bilingual language production. Neuropsychologia. 48: 1356-66. PMID 20036679 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2009.12.038 |
0.647 |
|
2010 |
Sandoval TC, Gollan TH, Ferreira VS, Salmon DP. What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy Bilingualism. 13: 231-252. DOI: 10.1017/S1366728909990514 |
0.659 |
|
2009 |
Bialystok E, Craik FI, Green DW, Gollan TH. Bilingual Minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest : a Journal of the American Psychological Society. 10: 89-129. PMID 26168404 DOI: 10.1177/1529100610387084 |
0.399 |
|
2009 |
Pyers JE, Gollan TH, Emmorey K. Bimodal bilinguals reveal the source of tip-of-the-tongue states. Cognition. 112: 323-9. PMID 19477437 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2009.04.007 |
0.656 |
|
2009 |
Gollan TH, Ferreira VS. Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 35: 640-65. PMID 19379041 DOI: 10.1037/A0014981 |
0.623 |
|
2009 |
Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Green DW, Gollan TH. Bilingual minds Psychological Science in the Public Interest, Supplement. 10: 89-129. DOI: 10.1177/1529100610387084 |
0.399 |
|
2008 |
Gollan TH, Montoya RI, Cera C, Sandoval TC. More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language. 58: 787-814. PMID 19343088 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2007.07.001 |
0.592 |
|
2008 |
Emmorey K, Borinstein HB, Thompson R, Gollan TH. Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England). 11: 43-61. PMID 19079743 DOI: 10.1017/S1366728907003203 |
0.577 |
|
2008 |
Rivera Mindt M, Arentoft A, Kubo Germano K, D'Aquila E, Scheiner D, Pizzirusso M, Sandoval TC, Gollan TH. Neuropsychological, cognitive, and theoretical considerations for evaluation of bilingual individuals. Neuropsychology Review. 18: 255-68. PMID 18841477 DOI: 10.1007/S11065-008-9069-7 |
0.524 |
|
2007 |
Gollan TH, Fennema-Notestine C, Montoya RI, Jernigan TL. The bilingual effect on Boston Naming Test performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : Jins. 13: 197-208. PMID 17286875 DOI: 10.1017/S1355617707070038 |
0.666 |
|
2007 |
Gollan TH, Fennema-Notestine C. What is it about bilingualism that affects Boston Naming Test performance? A reply to commentaries Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 13: 215-218. DOI: 10.1017/S1355617707070427 |
0.317 |
|
2006 |
Gollan TH, Brown AS. From tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) data to theoretical implications in two steps: when more TOTs means better retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 135: 462-83. PMID 16846276 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.462 |
0.423 |
|
2006 |
Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Paxton JL. Word association in early Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language. 99: 289-303. PMID 16122782 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandl.2005.07.001 |
0.42 |
|
2006 |
Finkbeiner M, Gollan TH, Caramazza A. Lexical access in bilingual speakers: What's the (hard) problem? Bilingualism. 9: 153-166. DOI: 10.1017/S1366728906002501 |
0.593 |
|
2005 |
Gollan TH, Montoya RI, Fennema-Notestine C, Morris SK. Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition. 33: 1220-34. PMID 16532855 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193224 |
0.601 |
|
2005 |
Thompson R, Emmorey K, Gollan TH. "Tip of the fingers" experiences by deaf signers: insights into the organization of a sign-based lexicon. Psychological Science. 16: 856-60. PMID 16262769 DOI: 10.1111/J.1467-9280.2005.01626.X |
0.638 |
|
2005 |
Gollan TH, Montoya RI, Bonanni MP. Proper names get stuck on bilingual and monolingual speakers' tip of the tongue equally often. Neuropsychology. 19: 278-87. PMID 15910114 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.278 |
0.598 |
|
2004 |
Gollan TH, Acenas LA. What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and tagalog-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 30: 246-69. PMID 14736310 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.246 |
0.579 |
|
2002 |
Gollan TH, Montoya RI, Werner GA. Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology. 16: 562-76. PMID 12382994 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.562 |
0.65 |
|
2001 |
Gollan TH, Frost R. Two routes to grammatical gender: evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 30: 627-51. PMID 11913850 DOI: 10.1023/A:1014235223566 |
0.367 |
|
2001 |
Gollan TH, Silverberg NB. Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew–English bilinguals Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 4: 63-83. DOI: 10.1017/S136672890100013X |
0.593 |
|
1997 |
Gollan TH, Forster KI, Frost R. Translation priming with different scripts: masked priming with cognates and noncognates in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 23: 1122-39. PMID 9293625 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.23.5.1122 |
0.701 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |